It's not unusual for people to use war metaphors such as "fight" and "battle" when trying to motivate patients with cancer.
But a new University of Michigan study indicates that using those words can have an unintended negative effect.
David Hauser, a U-M doctoral student in psychology, and colleague
Norbert Schwarz of the University of Southern California, found in three
studies that exposure to metaphoric language relating cancer to an
enemy significantly lessens the extent to which people consider
cancer-prevention behaviors.
"Hearing metaphoric utterances is enough to change the way we think
about a concept," said Hauser, the study's lead investigator. "When we
hear the phrase 'win the battle against cancer,' it forces us to think
of cancer as if it's an enemy that we are at war with."
These metaphors emphasize power and taking aggressive actions toward
an enemy. However, the bulk of cancer prevention behaviors -- such as
curbing alcohol intake, salty foods and smoking -- involve limitation
and restraint. None of them fit with an enemy metaphor that promotes
power and aggression, the researchers said.
"Hence, enemy metaphors de-emphasize this subset of beneficial
prevention behaviors and hurts people's willingness to engage in them,"
he said.
In one study, the authors asked participants to list
cancer-prevention behaviors they would be willing to undertake. For one
group of participants, the request contained metaphors relating cancer
to an enemy ("What things would you do to fight against developing
cancer?"). For a second group, the request contained no metaphors. The
group exposed to the enemy metaphor listed significantly less
limitation-related prevention behaviors.
"This suggests that simply seeing war metaphors for cancer diminishes
the extent to which these behaviors come to mind," Hauser said.
In another study, 313 participants read one of two health information
passages about colorectal cancer. One passage contained metaphors
relating cancer to an enemy ("This disease involves an enemy uprising of
abnormal cellular growth in the large intestine."), whereas the second
passage contained no enemy metaphors.
Participants then rated the extent to which they intended to engage
in various prevention, screening and treatment behaviors. Those who read
the enemy metaphor passage had less intention to engage in
limitation-related prevention behaviors (such as limiting red meat
intake or excessive alcohol consumption) than the participants who read
the second passage.
"Enemy metaphoric language for cancer diminishes people's intentions
for these types of prevention behaviors," Schwarz said. "Importantly,
these negative effects of enemy metaphors on prevention behaviors are
not accompanied by a positive effect on intentions to undertake
screening or treatment behaviors."
War and enemy metaphors are the most common metaphors found in
science journalism about cancer, and they pervade public discourse about
the disease.
"Fight and battle are actually among the top 10 verbs used to
describe cancer," Hauser said. "Constant exposure to even minor
metaphorical utterances may be enough to make enemy metaphors for cancer
a powerful influence on public health -- with unfortunate
side-effects."
The findings appear in the January issue of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
No comments:
Post a Comment